參考文獻 | 李仁豪(民88)。視覺注意力能否分裂。國立台灣大學心理學研究所碩士論文,未
出版,台北市。
李宏鎰(民83)。立體擷取之因素探討。私立輔仁大學應用心理學研究所碩士論文,
未出版,台北縣。
陳上文(民87)。視覺空間注意力的選擇基礎。國立中正大學心理學研究所博士論文,
未出版,嘉義縣。
劉英茂(民89)。基本心理歷程。台北市:文笙。
Aks, D. J. & Enns, J. T. (1996). Visual search for size is influenced by a background texture gradient. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22(6), 1467-1481.
Allport, D. A. (1971). Parallel encoding within and between elementary stimulus
dimensions. Perception & Psychophysics, 10, 104-108.
Andersen, G. J. (1990). Focused attention in three-dimensional space. Perception &
Psychophysics, 47 (1), 112-120.
Andersen, G. J. & Kramer, A. F. (1993). Limits of focused attention in three-
dimensional space. Perception & Psychophysics, 53(6), 658-667.
Anderson, B. L. (1998). Stereovision: beyond disparity computations. Trends in
Cognitive Science, 2(6), 214-222.
Anderson, B. L. (2003). The role of occlusion in the perception of depth , lightness, and
opacity. Psychological Review, 110(4), 785-801.
Anderson, B. L., Singh, M. & Fleming, R. W. (2002). The interpolation of object and
surface structure. Cognitive Psychology, 44, 148-190.
Anderson, B. L. & Julesz, B. (1995). A theoretical analysis of illusory contour formation
in stereopsis. Psychological Review, 102(4), 705-743.
Arnott, S. R. & Shedden, J. M. (2000). Attention switching in depth using random-dot
autostereograms: Attention gradient asymmetries. Perception & Psychophysics,
62(7), 1459-1473.
Atchley, P. & Kramer, A. F. (2001). Object and space-based attentional selection in
three-dimensional space. Visual Cognition, 8(1), 1-32.
Atchley, P., Kramer, A. F., Andersen, G. J., & Theeuwes, J. (1997). Spatial cuing in a
stereoscopic display: Evidence for a “depth-aware” attentional spotlight.
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4, 524–529.
Barlow, H. B., Blakemore, C. & Pettigrew, J. D. (1967). The neural mechanism of
binocular depth discrimination. Journal of Physiology, 193(2), 327–342.
Bashinski, H. S. & Bacharach, V. R. (1980). Enhancement of perceptual sensitivity as
the result of selectively attending to spatial location. Perception & Psychophysics,
28, 241-248.
Baylis, G. C., Driver, J. & McLeod, (1992). Visual parsing and response competition:
The effect of grouping factors. Perception & Psychophysics, 51, 145-162.
Baylis, G. C., & Driver, J. (1993). Visual attention and objects: evidence for
hierarchical coding of location. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19(3), 451–470.
Behrmann, M., & Tipper, S. P. (1994). Object-based attentional mechanisms: Evidence
from patients with unilateral neglect. In C. Umilta & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and Performance XV: Conscious and nonconscious information processing, 351-375. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Behrmann, M. & Tipper, S. (1996). Object-center not scene-based visual neglect.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22(5),
1261–1278.
Berhmann, M., & Tipper, S. (1999). Attention accesses multiple references frames:
evidence from unilateral neglect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 83–101.
Behrmann, M., Zemel, R. S. & Mozer, M. C. (1998). Object-based attention and
occlusion: Evidence from normal participants and a computational model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(4), 1011–1036.
Bichot, N. P., Cave, K. R., & Pashler, H. (1999). Visual selection mediated by location:
Feature-based selection of noncontiguous locations. Perception & Psychophysics,
61, 403-423.
Bisiach, E. & Luzzatti, C. (1978). Unilateral neglect of representational
space.Cortex,14, 129-133
Breckon. T. P. & Fisher, R. B. (2005). Amodal volume completion: 3D visual
completion. Computer Vision and Image Understanding, 99, 499-526.
Bressan, P., Mingolla, E., Spillmann, L. & Watanabe, T. (1997). Neon color spreading a
review. Perception, 26, 1353-1366.
Broadbent, D. (1958). Perception and communication. London: Pergamon Press.
Broadbent, D. (1982). Task combination and selective intake of information. Acta
Psychologics, 50, 253-290.
Castiello, U., & Umilt?, C. (1990). Size of the attentional focus and efficiency of
processing. Acta Psychologica, 73, 195-209.
Castiello, U., & Umilt?, C. (1992). Splitting focal attention. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18 (3), 837-848.
Cave, K. R., & Pashler, H. (1995). Visual selection mediated by location: Selecting
successive visual objects. Perception & Psychophysics, 57, 421-432.
Cave, K. R. & Bichot, N. P. (1999). Visuospatial attention: Beyond a spotlight model.
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 6(2), 204-223.
Cave, K. R. & Kosslyn, S. M. (1989). Varieties of size-specific visual selection. Journal
of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 148-164.
Cave, K. R. & Zimmerman, J. M. (1997), Flexibility in spatial attention before and after
practice. Psychological Science, 8, 399-403.
Cepeda, N. J., Cave, K. R., Bichot, N. P., & Kim, M.-S. (1998). Spatial selection via
feature-driven inhibition of distractor locations. Perception & Psychophysics, 60,
727-746.
Chastain, G. (1992a). Analog versus discrete shifts of attention across the visual field.
Psychological Research, 54, 175-181
Chastain, G. (1992b). Time-course of sensitivity changes as attention shifts to an
unpredictable location. Journal of General Psychology, 119, 105-111.
Conci, M. (2005). Figural completion in visual search. 民國95年3月23日,取自:
http://edoc.ub.uni-muenchen.de/archive/00004067/01/Conci_Markus.pdf
Chaudhuri, A. (1990). Modulation of the motion aftereffect by selective attention.
Nature, 344, 60-62.
Chun, M. M. & Wolfe, J. M. (2000). Visual attention. In E. B. Goldstein (Eds.),
Blackwell Handbook of Perception. Blackwell Publishing.
Davis, G., & Driver, J. (1994). Parallel detection of Kanizsa subjective figures in the
human visual system. Nature, 371 (6500), 791-793.
Desimone, R. & Duncan, J. (1995) Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention.
Annual Review Neuroscience, 18, 193-222.
Downing, C. J. (1988) .Expectancy and visual spatial attention: Effects on perceptual
quality. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 188-202.
Downing, C. J. & Pinker, S. (1985). The spatial structure of visual attention. In M. I.
Posner & S. M. Mann (Eds.), Attention and Performance XI. Hillsdale, N. J.: L.
Erlbaum Associates.
Driver, J., & Baylis, G. C. (1989). Movement and visual attention: The spotlight
metaphor breaks down. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception
and Performance, 15 (3), 448-456.
Driver, J. & Halligan, P. W. (1991). Can visual neglect operate in object-centred
co-ordinates? an affirmative single-case study. Cognitive neuropsychology, 8(66), 475-496.
Duncan, J. (1984). Selective attention and the organization of visual information.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 4, 501-517.
Eriksen, C. W., & Hoffman, J. E. (1973). The extent of processing of noise elements
during selective encoding from visual displays. Perception & Psychophysics, 14
(1), 155-160.
Eriksen, C. W., & St James, J. D. (1986). Visual attention within and round the field of
focal attention: A zoom lens model. Perception & Psychophysics, 40, 225–240.
Eriksen, C. W., & Webb, J. M. (1989). Shifting of attentional focus within and about a
visual display. Perception & Psychophysics, 45, 175-183.
Eriksen, C. W., & Yeh, Y. Y. (1985). Allocation of attention in the visual field. Journal
of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11(5), 583-597.
Egeth, H. (1977). Attention and preattention. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of
learning and motivation, 11, pp. 277-320. New York: Academic Press.
Egly, R., Driver, J., & Rafal, R.D. (1994). Shifting visual attention between objects and
location: Evidence from normal and parietal lesion subjects. Journal of
Experimental Psychology. General, 123, 16 1-177.
Egly, R., & Homa, D. (1984). Sensitization of the visual field. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10 (6), 778-793.
Fortin, A., Ptito, A., Faubert, J. & Ptito, M. (2002). Cortical areas mediating stereopsis
in the human brain: a PET study. Neuroreport, 13(6), 895-898.
Fink, G.R., Dolan, R. J., Halligan, P. W., Marshall, J.C. & Frith, C.D. (1997). Space-
based and object-based visual attention: shared and specific neural domains. Brain, 120, 2013–2028.
Gatti, S. V., & Egeth, H. E. (1978). Failure of spatial selectivity in vision. Bulletin of the
Psychonomic Society, 11, 181-184.
Gawryszewski, L. D. G., Riggio, L., Rizzolatti, G., & Umilt?, C. (1987). Movements of
attention in three spatial dimensions and the meaning of “neutral” cues. Neuropsychologia, 25, 19–29.
Gibson, E. J. (1969). Principles of perceptual learning and development. New York:
Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Grabrowecky, M. & Treisman, A. (1989). Attention and fixation in subjective contours
perception. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, Supplement, 30, 457.
Greenwald, A. G. (1972). On doing two things at once: Time sharing as a function of
ideomotor compatibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 94(1), 52-57.
Han, S., Wan X. & Humphreys, G. W. (2005). Shifts of spatial attention in perceived
3-D space. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58(A), 753-764.
Handy, T. C., Kingstone, A., & Mangun, G. R. (1996). Spatial distribution of visual
attention: Perceptual sensitivity and response latency. Perception & Psychophysics,
58, 613-627.
He, Z. J., & Nakayama, K. (1995). Visual attention to surfaces in three-dimensional
space. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 92, 11155–11159.
Heinze, H.-J., Luck, S. J., M?nte, T. F., G?s, A., Mangun, G. R., & Hillyard, S. A.
(1994). Attention to adjacent and separate positions in space: An electrophysiological analysis. Perception & Psychophysics, 56, 42-52.
Henderson, J. M. & Macquistan, A. D. (1993). The spatial distribution of attention
following an exogenous cue. Perception & Psychophysics, 53(2), 221-230.
Howard, I. P. & Rogers, B. J. (2002). Seeing in depth. Vol 2: Depth perception. I.
, Toronto: Porteous.
Hoffman, J. E., & Nelson, B. (1981). Spatial selectivity in visual search. Perception &
Psychophysics, 30, 283-290.
Hubel, D. H. & Wiesel. T. N. (1959). Receptive fields of single neurons in the cat's
striate cortex. Journal of Physiology, 148(3), 574–591.
Hubel, D. H. & Wiesel. T. N. (1962). Receptive fields, binocular interaction and
functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex. Journal of Physiology, 148(3), 574–591.
Iavecchia, H. P., & Folk, C. L. (1994). Shifting visual attention in stereographic displays: A time course analysis. Human Factors, 36, 606–618.
Johnson, D. N. & Yantis, S. (1995). Allocating Visual Attention: Tests of a Two-Process
Model. Journal of ExperimentalPsychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(6), 1376-1390.
Jonides, J. (1983). Further toward a model of the mind's eye's movement. Bulletin of the
Psychonomic Society. 21(4), 247-250.
Juola, J. F., Bouwhuis, D. G., Cooper, E. E., & Warner, B. (1991). Control of attention
around the fovea. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17(1), 125-141.
Kahneman, D., & Henik, A. (1977). Effects of visual grouping on immediate recall and
selective attention. In S. Dornic (Ed.), Attention and Performance VII. Hillsdale, N. J. Eribaum.
Kahneman, D., & Henik, A. (1981). Perceptual organization and attention. In M.
Kubovy & J. R. Pomerantz (Eds), Perceptual organization. Hillsdale, N. J.: Erlbaum.
Kahneman, D., & Treisman A. M. (1984).Changing views of attention and automaticity.
In R. Parasuraman, R. & D. R. Davies(Eds.), Varieties of attention, (pp.29-61). New York: Academic Press.
Kasai, T., Morotomi, T., Katayama, J. & Kumada, T. (2003). Attending to a location in
three-dimensional space modulates early ERPs. Cognitive Brain Research, 17, 273-285.
Klein, R., & McCormick, P. (1989). Covert visual orienting: Hemifield-activation can
be mimicked by zoom lens and midlocation placement strategies. Acta
Psychologica, 70, 235-250.
Kim, M. S. & Cave, K. R. (1995), Spatial attention in search for features and feature
conjunctions. Psychonomic Science , 6, 376–380.
Kramer, A. F., & Hahn, 5. (1995). Splitting the beam: Distribution of attention over
noncontiguous regions of the visual field. Psychological Science, 6(6), 38 1-386.
Kramer, A. F., & Jacobson, A. (1991). Perceptual organization and focused attention:
The role of objects and proximity in visual processing. Perception & Psychophysics, 50 (3), 267-284.
Kramer, A. F., Tham, M. P., & Yeh, Y. Y. (1991). Movement and focused attention: A
failure to replicate. Perception & Psychophysics, 50 (6), 5 37-546.
Krose, B. J. A., & Julesz, B. (1989). The control and speed of shifts of attention. Vision
Research, 29 (11), 1607-1619.
Kwak, H-W., Dagenbach, D., & Egeth, H. (1991). Further evidence for a time-
independent shift of the focus of attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 49 (5), 473-480.
LaBerge, D. (1983). Spatial extent of attention to letters and words. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9, 371–379.
LaBerge, D., & Brown, V. (1986). Variations in size of the visual field in which targets
are presented: An attentional range effect. Perception & Psychophysics, 40, 188- 200.
LaBerge, D., & Brown, V. (1989). Theory of attention operations in shape identification.
Psychological Review, 96 (1), 10 1-124.
Lamy, D. & Tsal, Y. (2001). On the status of location in visual attention. European
Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 13(3), 305-342.
Larsen, A., & Bundesen, C. (1978). Size scaling in visual pattern recognition. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 4, 1-20.
Lavie, N. (1995). Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(3),
451-468.
Lavie, N., & Driver, J. (1996). On the spatial extent of attention in object-based visual
selection. Perception & Psychophysics, 58 (8), 1238-1251.
Li, C. Y. & Guo, K. (1995). Measurements of geometric illusions, illusory contours and
stereo-depth at luminance and colour contrast. Vision Research, 35(12), 1713-1720.
Logan, G. D. (1996). The CODE theory of visual attention: An Integration of space-
based and object-based attention. Psychological Review, 103(4), 603-649.
Luck, S. J., Hillyard, S. A., Mouloua, M. & Hawkins, H. L. (1996). Mechanisms of
visual-spatial attention: Resource allocation or uncertainty reduction? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22(3), 725-737.
Mangun, G. R., & Hillyard, S. A. (1995). Mechanisms and models of selective
attention. In M. D. Rugg & M. G. H. Coles (Eds.), Electrophysiology of mind: Event-related brain potentials and cognition (pp. 41-85). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Marrara, M. T., & Moore, C. M. (2000). Role of perceptual organization while attending
in depth. Perception & Psychophysics, 62, 786–799.
McAllister, D. F. (2002). 3D Displays. Wiley Encyclopedia on Imaging, 1327-1344.
Motter, B. C. (1994). Neural correlates of feature selective memory and pop-out in
extrastriate area V4. Journal of Neuroscience, 14(4), 2190-2199.
McCormick, P. A., Klein, R., & Johnston, S. (1998). Splitting versus sharing focal
attention: Comment on Cattiello and Umilta (1992). Journal of Experimental 78
Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24 (1), 350-357.
Morita, H., Morita, M. & Kumada, T. (2003). Integration processof contours defined by
different attributes. Cognitive Brain Research, 15, 324-327.
Murphy, T. D., & Eriksen, C. W. (1987). Temporal changes in the distribution of
attention in the visual field in response to precues. Perception & Psychophysics, 42, 576-586.
Muller, H. J. & Findlay, J. M. (1987). Sensitivity and criterion effects in the spatial
cuing of visual attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 42, 383-399.
Naatanen, R. (1992). Attention and Brain Function, Hillsdale, NJ.: L. Erlbaum. Ch. 2.
Nakayama, K., Shimojo, S. & Silverman, G. H.(1989) Stereoscopic depth: Its relation to
image segmentation, grouping, and the recognition of occluded objects. Perception, 18, 55-68.
Pan, K., & Eriksen, C. W. (1993). Attentional distribution in the visual field during
same–different judgments as assessed by response competition. Perception &
Psychophysics, 53, 134-144.
Pashler, H., & Johnson, J. C. (1998). Attention limitations in dual-task performance. In
H. Pashler (Ed.). Attention. NY: Psychology press.
Pettigrew, T., Nikara, P. O. & Bishop, J. D. (1968). Analysis of retinal correspondence
by studying receptive fields of binocular single units in cat striate cortex.
Experimental Brain Research, 6(4), 353-372.
Podgorny, R., & Shepard, R. N. (1983). Distribution of visual attention over space.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 9,
380-393.
Pollack, J. (2004). 3D display technology from stereoscope to autostereo displays. 民
95年2月19日,取自:http://www.sidchapters.org/ pacificnorthwest/ meetings/ aug25_04_presentation.pdf#search='sharp%203D%20display%20technology%20Pollack'.
Posner, M. I., Nissen, M. J. & Ogden, W. C. (1978). Attended and unattended
processing modes: The role of set for spatial location. In: Modes of Perceiving and
Processing Information. Hillsdale, N. J. : Erlbaum.
Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R. R., & Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of
signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 160-174.
Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 32, 3-25.
Posner, M. I. & Cohen, Y. (1980). Attention and the control of movements. In G. E.
Stelmach & J. Requin (Eds.), Tutorials in motor behavior. Amsterdam: North Holland Press, 243–258.
Ramachandran, V. S. (1986c). Capture of stereopsis and apparent motion by illusory
contours. Perception & Psychophysics, 39, 361-373.
Ramachandran, V. S. & Cavanagh, P. (1985). Subjective contours capture stereopsis.
Nature, 317, 527-530.
Rafal, R. D., Calabresi, P. A., Brennan, C. W. & Sciolto, T. K. (1989). Saccade
preparation inhibits reorienting to recently attended locations. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15(4), 673-685.
Remington, R., & Pierce, L. (1984). Moving attention: Evidence for time-invariant
shifts of visual selective attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 35, 393-399.
Ricciardelli, P., Bonfiglioli, C., Nicoletti, R. & Umilt?, C. (2001). Focusing attention on
overlapping and non-overlapping figures with subjective contours. Psychological Research, 65, 98-106.
Sagi, D., & Julesz, B. (1985). Fast noninertial shifts of attention. Spatial Vision, 1, 141-
149.
Scholl, B. J. (2001). Objects and attention: the state of the art. Cognition, 80, 1-46.
Schall, J. D., Morel, A., King, D. J. & Bullier, J. (1995). Topography of visual cortical
afferents of frontal eye field in macaque: functional convergence and segregation of processing streams. Journal of Neuroscience, 15, 4464-4487.
Skelton, J. M. & Eriksen, C. W. (1976). Spatial characteristics of selective attention in
letter matching. Bull Psychonomic Society, 7, 136–138.
Sperling, G. & Weichselgartner, E. (1995). Episodic theory of the dynamics of spatial
attention. Psychological Review, 102(3), 503-532.
Shaw, M. L., & Shaw, P. (1977). Optimal allocation of cognitive resources to spatial
locations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 3 (2), 201-211.
Shaw, M. L. (1978). A capacity allocation model for reaction time. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4, 586-598.
Shipley T. F. & Kellman, P. J. (1996). Strength of visual interpolation depends on the
ratio of physically specified to total edge length. Perception & Psychophysics,
52(1), 97-106.
Shulman, G. L., Remington, R. W., & McLean, J. P. (1979). Moving attention through
physical space. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 5, 522-526.
Theeuwes, J. (1995). Abrupt luminance change pop out: Abrupt olor change does not.
Perception and Psychophysics, 57, 637-644.
Theeuwes, J., Atchley, P., & Kramer, A. F. (1998). Attentional control within 3-D space.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1476–1485.
Theeuwes, J., Kramer, A. F., & Atchley, P. (1999). Attentional effects on preattentive
vision: Spatial precues affect the detection of simple features. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25(2), 341-347.
Tipper, S. P., Driver, J. & Weaver, B. (1991). Object-centred inhibition of return of
visual attention. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 43(2), 289-298.
Tipper, S. P., Weaver, B., Jerraut, L. M. & Burak, A. L. (1994). Object-based amd
environment-based inhibition of return of visual attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(3), 478-499.
Treisman, A. M. (1960). Contextual cues in selective listening. Quarterly Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 12, 242-248.
Treisman, A. (1969). Strategies and models of selective attention.
Psychological Review, 76 (3), 2 82-299.
Treisman, A. (1988). Features and objects: The fourteen Bartlett memorial lecture.
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40A(2), 201-237.
Trick, L. M., & Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1994). Why are small and large numbers enumerated
differently? A limited-capacity preattentive stage in vision. Psychological Review, 101(1), 80-102.
Tsal, Y. (1983). Movements of attention across the visual field. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9, 523-530.
Tsal, Y. (1994). Effects of attention on perception of features and figural organisation.
Perception, 23, 441-452.
Tsal, Y., & Lavie, N. (1993). Location dominance in attending to color and shape.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19 (1), 131-139.
Uttal, W. R., Davis, N.S. & Welke, C. (1994). Stereoscopic perception with brief
exposures. Perception & Psychophysics, 56(5), 599-604.
van der Heijden, A. H. C, Schreuder, R., Maris, L., & Neerincx, M. (1984). Some
evidence for correlated separate activation in a simple letter-detection task. Perception & Psychophysics, 36, 577-585.
van der Helden, J. & Wijers, B. (2005). Two alternative target locations and object-
based attention in Kanizsa Illusory and Amodal objects.民95年3月25日,取自:http://dissertations.ub.rug.nl/FILES/faculties/ppsw/2005/j.van.der.helden/c2.pdf
Vecera, S. P., & Farah, M. J. (1994). Does visual attention select objects or locations?
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123 (2), 146-160.
Vreven, D. & Welch, L. (2001). The absence of depth constancy in contour stereograms,
Perception, 30, 693-705.
Watanabe, T. & Cavanagh, P. (1992). Depth capture and transparency of regions
bounded by illusory and chromatic contours. Vision Research, 32(30), 527-532.
Watt, R. J. (1988). Visual processing: computational, psychophysical, and cognitive
research. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Watson, S. E., & Kramer, A. F. (1999). Object-based visual selective attention and
perceptual organization. Perception & Psychophysics, 61(1), 31-49.
Wing, A. & Allport, D. A. (1972). Multidimensional encoding of visual form.
Perception & Psychophysics, 12, 474–476.
Yantis, S. (1988). On analog movements of visual attention. Perception &
Psychophysics, 43, 203-206.
Yantis, S. (1995). Perceived continuity of occluded visual objects. Psychological
Science, 6, 182-186.
Yeh, S., Chen, I., De Valois, K. K., & De Valois, R. L. (1996). Figural aflereffects and
spatial attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22 (2), 446-460.
|